Why
do we do the things we do? Despite our best attempts to "know thyself,"
the truth is that we often know astonishingly little about our own
minds, and even less about the way others think. As Charles Dickens once
put it, “A wonderful fact to reflect upon, that every human creature is
constituted to be that profound secret and mystery to every other.”
Psychologists
have long sought insights into how we perceive the world and what
motivates our behavior, and they've made enormous strides in lifting
that veil of mystery. Aside from providing fodder for stimulating
cocktail-party conversations, some of the most famous psychological
experiments of the past century reveal universal and often surprising
truths about human nature. Here are 10 classic psychological studies that may change the way you understand yourself.
We all have some capacity for evil.
Arguably
the most famous experiment in the history of psychology, the 1971
Stanford prison study put a microscope on how social situations can
affect human behavior. The researchers, led by psychologist Philip
Zimbardo, set up a mock prison in the basement of the Stanford psych
building and selected 24 undergraduates (who had no criminal record and
were deemed psychologically healthy) to act as prisoners and guards.
Researchers then observed the prisoners (who had to stay in the cells 24
hours a day) and guards (who shared eight-hour shifts) using hidden
cameras.
The
experiment, which was scheduled to last for two weeks, had to be cut
short after just six days due to the guards' abusive behavior -- in some
cases they even inflicted psychological torture -- and the extreme
emotional stress and anxiety exhibited by the prisoners.
"The
guards escalated their aggression against the prisoners, stripping them
naked, putting bags over their heads, and then finally had them engage
in increasingly humiliating sexual activities," Zimbardo told American
Scientist. "After six days I had to end it because it was out of control
-- I couldn't really go to sleep at night without worrying what the
guards could do to the prisoners."
We don't notice what's right in front of us.
Think
you know what's going on around you? You might not be nearly as aware
as you think. In 1998, researchers from Harvard and Kent State
University targeted pedestrians on a college campus to determine how
much people notice about their immediate environments. In the
experiment, an actor came up to a pedestrian and asked for directions.
While the pedestrian was giving the directions, two men carrying a large
wooden door walked between the actor and the pedestrian, completely
blocking their view of each other for several seconds. During that time,
the actor was replaced by another actor, one of a different height and
build, and with a different outfit, haircut and voice. A full half of
the participants didn't notice the substitution.
The
experiment was one of the first to illustrate the phenomenon of "change
blindness," which shows just how selective we are about what we take in
from any given visual scene -- and it seems that we rely on memory and
pattern-recognition significantly more than we might think.
Delaying gratification is hard -- but we're more successful when we do.
A
famous Stanford experiment from the late 1960s tested preschool
children's ability to resist the lure of instant gratification -- and it
yielded some powerful insights about willpower and self-discipline. In
the experiment, four-year-olds were put in a room by themselves with a
marshmallow on a plate in front of them, and told that they could either
eat the treat now, or if they waited until the researcher returned 15
minutes later, they could have two marshmallows.
While
most of the children said they'd wait, they often struggled to resist
and then gave in, eating the treat before the researcher returned, TIME
reports. The children who did manage to hold off for the full 15 minutes
generally used avoidance tactics, like turning away or covering their
eyes. The implications of the children's behavior were significant:
Those who were able to delay gratification were much less likely to be
obese, or to have drug addiction or behavioral problems by the time they
were teenagers, and were more successful later in life.
We can experience deeply conflicting moral impulses.
A
famous 1961 study by Yale psychologist Stanley Milgram tested (rather
alarmingly) how how far people would go to obey authority figures when
asked to harm others, and the intense internal conflict between personal
morals and the obligation to obey authority figures.
Milgram
wanted to conduct the experiment to provide insight into how Nazi war
criminals could have perpetuated unspeakable acts during the Holocaust.
To do so, he tested a pair of participants, one deemed the "teacher" and
the other deemed the "learner." The teacher was instructed to
administer electric shocks to the learner (who was supposedly sitting in
another room, but in reality was not being shocked) each time they got
questions wrong. Milgram instead played recordings which made it sound
like the learner was in pain, and if the "teacher" subject expressed a
desire to stop, the experimenter prodded him to go on. During the first
experiment, 65 percent of participants administered a painful, final
450-volt shock (labeled "XXX"), although many were visibly stressed and
uncomfortable about doing so.
While
the study has commonly been seen as a warning of blind obedience to
authority, Scientific American recently revisited it, arguing that the
results were more suggestive of deep moral conflict.
"Human
moral nature includes a propensity to be empathetic, kind and good to
our fellow kin and group members, plus an inclination to be xenophobic,
cruel and evil to tribal others," journalist Michael Shermer wrote. "The
shock experiments reveal not blind obedience but conflicting moral
tendencies that lie deep within."
Recently,
some commenters have called Milgram's methodology into question, and
one critic noted that records of the experiment performed at Yale
suggested that 60 percent of participants actually disobeyed orders to
administer the highest-dosage shock.
We're easily corrupted by power.
There's
a psychological reason behind the fact that those in power sometimes
act towards others with a sense of entitlement and disrespect. A 2003
study published in the journal Psychological Review put students into
groups of three to write a short paper together. Two students were
instructed to write the paper, while the other was told to evaluate the
paper and determine how much each student would be paid. In the middle
of their work, a researcher brought in a plate of five cookies. Although
generally the last cookie was never eaten, the "boss" almost always ate
the fourth cookie -- and ate it sloppily, mouth open.
"When
researchers give people power in scientific experiments, they are more
likely to physically touch others in potentially inappropriate ways, to
flirt in more direct fashion, to make risky choices and gambles, to make
first offers in negotiations, to speak their mind, and to eat cookies
like the Cookie Monster, with crumbs all over their chins and
chests," psychologist Dacher Keltner, one of the study's leaders, wrote
in an article for UC Berkeley's Greater Good Science Center.
We seek out loyalty to social groups and are easily drawn to intergroup conflict.
This
classic 1950s social psychology experiment shined a light on the
possible psychological basis of why social groups and countries find
themselves embroiled in conflict with one another -- and how they can
learn to cooperate again.
Study
leader Muzafer Sherif took two groups of 11 boys (all age 11) to
Robbers Cave State Park in Oklahoma for "summer camp." The groups (named
the "Eagles" and the "Rattlers") spent a week apart, having fun
together and bonding, with no knowledge of the existence of the other
group. When the two groups finally integrated, the boys started calling
each other names, and when they started competing in various games, more
conflict ensued and eventually the groups refused to eat together. In
the next phase of the research, Sherif designed experiments to try to
reconcile the boys by having them enjoy leisure activities together
(which was unsuccessful) and then having them solve a problem together,
which finally began to ease the conflict.
We only need one thing to be happy.
The
75-year Harvard Grant study --one of the most comprehensive
longitudinal studies ever conducted -- followed 268 male Harvard
undergraduates from the classes of 1938-1940 (now well into their 90s)
for 75 years, regularly collecting data on various aspects of their
lives. The universal conclusion? Love really is all that matters, at least when it comes to determining long-term happiness and life satisfaction.
The
study's longtime director, psychiatrist George Vaillant, told The
Huffington Postthat there are two pillars of happiness: "One is love.
The other is finding a way of coping with life that does not push love
away." For example, one participant began the study with the lowest
rating for future stability of all the subjects and he had previously
attempted suicide. But at the end of his life, he was one of the
happiest. Why? As Vaillant explains, “He spent his life searching for
love.”
We thrive when we have strong self-esteem and social status.
Achieving
fame and success isn't just an ego boost -- it could also be a key to
longevity, according to the notorious Oscar winners study. Researchers
from Toronto's Sunnybrook and Women's College Health Sciences Centre
found that Academy Award-winning actors and directors tend to live
longer than those who were nominated but lost, with winning actors and
actresses outliving their losing peers by nearly four years.
"We
are not saying that you will live longer if you win an Academy
Award," Donald Redelmeier, the lead author of the study, told ABC News.
"Or that people should go out and take acting courses. Our main
conclusion is simply that social factors are important ... It suggests
that an internal sense of self-esteem is an important aspect to health
and health care."
We constantly try to justify our experiences so that they make sense to us.
Anyone
who's taken a freshman Psych 101 class is familiar with cognitive
dissonance, a theory which dictates that human beings have a natural
propensity to avoid psychological conflict based on disharmonious or
mutually exclusive beliefs. In an often-cited 1959 experiment,
psychologist Leon Festinger asked participants to perform a series of
dull tasks, like turning pegs in a wooden knob, for an hour. They were
then paid either $1 or $20 to tell a "waiting participant" (aka a
researcher) that the task was very interesting. Those who were paid $1
to lie rated the tasks as more enjoyable than those who were paid $20.
Their conclusion? Those who were paid more felt that they had sufficient
justification for having performed the rote task for an hour, but those
who were only paid $1 felt the need to justify the time spent (and
reduce the level of dissonance between their beliefs and their behavior)
by saying that the activity was fun. In other words, we commonly tell
ourselves lies to make the world appear a more logical, harmonious
place.
We buy into stereotypes in a big way.
Stereotyping
various groups of people based on social group, ethnicity or class is
something nearly all of us do, even if we make an effort not to -- and
it can lead us to draw unfair and potentially damaging conclusions about
entire populations. NYU psychologist John Bargh's experiments
on "automaticity of social behavior" revealed that we often judge people
based on unconscious stereotypes -- and we can't help but act on them.
We also tend to buy into stereotypes for social groups that we see
ourselves being a part of. In one study, Bargh found that a group of
participants who were asked to unscramble words related to old age --
"Florida," "helpless" and "wrinkled" -- walked significantly slower down
the hallway after the experiment than the group who unscrambled words
unrelated to age. Bargh repeated the findings in two other comparable
studies that enforced stereotypes based on race and politeness.
"Stereotypes
are categories that have gone too far," Bargh told Psychology Today.
"When we use stereotypes, we take in the gender, the age, the color of
the skin of the person before us, and our minds respond with messages
that say hostile, stupid, slow, weak. Those qualities aren't out there
in the environment. They don't reflect reality."
No comments:
Post a Comment